Self-awareness
In Philosophy of self, self-awareness is the experience of one's own personality or individuality.[1][2] It is not to be confused with consciousness in the sense of qualia. While consciousness is being aware of one's environment and body and lifestyle, self-awareness is the recognition of that awareness.[3] Self-awareness is how an individual consciously knows and understands their own character, feelings, motives, and desires. There are two broad categories of self-awareness: internal self-awareness and external self-awareness.[4]
Neurobiological basis
Introduction
Body
There are questions regarding what part of the brain allows us to be self-aware and how we are biologically programmed to be self-aware. V.S. Ramachandran has speculated that mirror neurons may provide the neurological basis of human self-awareness.[5] In an essay written for the Edge Foundation in 2009, Ramachandran gave the following explanation of his theory: "... I also speculated that these neurons can not only help simulate other people's behavior but can be turned 'inward'—as it were—to create second-order representations or meta-representations of your own earlier brain processes. This could be the neural basis of introspection, and of the reciprocity of self awareness and other awareness. There is obviously a chicken-or-egg question here as to which evolved first, but... The main point is that the two co-evolved, mutually enriching each other to create the mature representation of self that characterizes modern humans."[6]
Bodily (self-)awareness is related to proprioception and visualization
Health
In health and medicine, body-awareness is a construct that refers to a person's overall ability to direct their focus on various internal sensations accurately. Both proprioception and interoception allow individuals to be consciously aware of various sensations.[7] Proprioception allows individuals and patients to focus on sensations in their muscles and joints, posture, and balance, while interoception is used to determine sensations of the internal organs, such as fluctuating heartbeat, respiration, lung pain, or satiety. Over-acute body-awareness, under-acute body-awareness, and distorted body-awareness are symptoms present in a variety of health disorders and conditions, such as obesity, anorexia nervosa, and chronic joint pain.[8] For example, a distorted perception of satiety present in a patient suffering from anorexia nervosa
Human development
Bodily self-awareness in human development refers to one's awareness of their body as a physical object, with physical properties, that can interact with other objects. Tests have shown that at the age of only a few months old, toddlers are already aware of the relationship between the proprioceptive and visual information they receive.[9] This is called first-person self-awareness.
At around 18 months old and later, children begin to develop reflective self-awareness, which is the next stage of bodily awareness and involves children recognizing themselves in reflections, mirrors, and pictures.[10] Children who have not obtained this stage of bodily self-awareness yet will tend to view reflections of themselves as other children and respond accordingly, as if they were looking at someone else face to face. In contrast, those who have reached this level of awareness will recognize that they see themselves, for instance seeing dirt on their face in the reflection and then touching their own face to wipe it off.
Slightly after toddlers become reflectively self-aware, they begin to develop the ability to recognize their bodies as physical objects in time and space that interact and impact other objects. For instance, a toddler placed on a blanket, when asked to hand someone the blanket, will recognize that they need to get off it to be able to lift it.[9] This is the final stage of body self-awareness and is called objective self-awareness.
Non-human animals
Studies have been done mainly on primates to test if self-awareness is present. Apes, monkeys, elephants, and dolphins have been studied most frequently. The most relevant studies to this day that represent self-awareness in animals have been done on chimpanzees, dolphins, and magpies. Self-awareness in animals is tested through mirror self-recognition.
Animals that show mirror self-recognition undergo four stages:
- social response,
- physical mirror inspection,
- repetitive mirror testing behavior, and
- the mark test, which involves the animals spontaneously touching a mark on their body which would have been difficult to see without the mirror.[11]
David DeGrazia states that there are three types of self-awareness in animals; the first being, bodily self-awareness. This sense of awareness allows animals to understand that they are different from the rest of the environment; it is also the reason why animals do not eat themselves. Bodily-awareness also includes proprioception and sensation. The second type of self-awareness in animals is social self-awareness. This type of awareness is seen in highly social animals and is the awareness that they have a role within themselves in order to survive. This type of awareness allows animals to interact with each other. The final type of self-awareness is introspective awareness. This awareness is responsible for animals to understand feelings, desires, and beliefs.[12]
The red-spot technique created and experimented by Gordon G. Gallup[13] studies self-awareness in animals (primates). In this technique, a red odorless spot is placed on an anesthetized primate's forehead. The spot is placed on the forehead so that it can only be seen through a mirror. Once the individual awakens, independent movements toward the spot after seeing their reflection in a mirror are observed. During the red-spot technique, after looking in the mirror, chimpanzees used their fingers to touch the red dot that was on their forehead and, after touching the red dot they would even smell their fingertips.[14] "Animals that can recognize themselves in mirrors can conceive of themselves," says Gallup. Another prime example are elephants. Three elephants were exposed to large mirrors where experimenters studied the reaction when the elephants saw their reflection. These elephants were given the "litmus mark test" in order to see whether they were aware of what they were looking at. This visible mark was applied on the elephants and the researchers reported a large progress with self-awareness. The elephants shared this success rate with other animals such as monkeys and dolphins.[15]
Chimpanzees and other apes – species which have been studied extensively – compare the most to humans with the most convincing findings and straightforward evidence in the relativity of self-awareness in animals so far.[16] Dolphins were put to a similar test and achieved the same results. Diana Reiss, a psycho-biologist at the New York Aquarium discovered that bottlenose dolphins can recognize themselves in mirrors.[17]
Researchers also used the mark test or mirror test[18] to study the magpie's self-awareness. As a majority of birds are blind below the beak, Prior et al.[16] marked the birds’ neck with three different colors: red, yellow, and black (as an imitation, as magpies are originally black). When placed in front of a mirror, the birds with the red and yellow spots began scratching at their necks, signaling the understanding of something different being on their bodies. During one trial with a mirror and a mark, three out of the five magpies showed a minimum of one example of self-directed behavior. The magpies explored the mirror by moving toward it and looking behind it. One of the magpies, Harvey, during several trials would pick up objects, pose, do some wing-flapping, all in front of the mirror with the objects in his beak. This represents a sense of self-awareness; knowing what is going on within himself and in the present. The authors suggest that self-recognition in birds and mammals may be a case of convergent evolution, where similar evolutionary pressures result in similar behaviors or traits, although they arrive at them via different routes.[19]
A few slight occurrences of behavior towards the magpie's own body happened in the trial with the black mark and the mirror. It is assumed in this study[16] that the black mark may have been slightly visible on the black feathers. Prior et al.[16] stated, "This is an indirect support for the interpretation that the behavior towards the mark region was elicited by seeing the own body in the mirror in conjunction with an unusual spot on the body."
The behaviors of the magpies clearly contrasted with no mirror present. In the no-mirror trials, a non-reflective gray plate of the same size and in the same position as the mirror was swapped in. There were not any mark directed self-behaviors when the mark was present, in color, or in black.[16] Prior's et al.[16] data quantitatively matches the findings in chimpanzees. In summary of the mark test,[16] the results show that magpies understand that a mirror image represents their own body; magpies show to have self-awareness.
Cooperation and evolutionary problems
An organism can be effectively altruistic without being self-aware, aware of any distinction between egoism and altruism, or aware of qualia in others. This by simple reactions to specific situations which happens to benefit other individuals in the organism's natural environment. If self-awareness led to a necessity of an emotional empathy mechanism for altruism and egoism being default in its absence, that would have precluded evolution from a state without self-awareness to a self-aware state in all social animals. The ability of the theory of evolution to explain self-awareness can be rescued by abandoning the hypothesis of self-awareness being a basis for cruelty.[20][21]
Psychology
Self-awareness has been called "arguably the most fundamental issue in psychology, from both a developmental and an evolutionary perspective."[22]
Self-awareness theory, developed by Duval and Wicklund in their 1972 landmark book A theory of objective self awareness, states that when we focus our attention on ourselves, we evaluate and compare our current behavior to our internal standards and values. This elicits a state of objective self-awareness. We become self-conscious as objective evaluators of ourselves.[23] However self-awareness is not to be confused with self-consciousness.[24] Various emotional states are intensified by self-awareness. However, some people may seek to increase their self-awareness through these outlets. People are more likely to align their behavior with their standards when made self-aware. People will be negatively affected if they don't live up to their personal standards. Various environmental cues and situations induce awareness of the self, such as mirrors, an audience, or being videotaped or recorded. These cues also increase accuracy of personal memory.[25] In one of Demetriou's neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development, self-awareness develops systematically from birth through the life span and it is a major factor for the development of general inferential processes.[26] Moreover, a series of recent studies showed that self-awareness about cognitive processes participates in general intelligence on a par with processing efficiency functions, such as working memory, processing speed, and reasoning.[27] Albert Bandura's theory of self-efficacy builds on our varying degrees of self-awareness. It is "the belief in one's capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to manage prospective situations." A person's belief in their ability to succeed sets the stage to how they think, behave and feel. Someone with a strong self-efficacy, for example, views challenges as mere tasks that must be overcome, and are not easily discouraged by setbacks. They are aware of their flaws and abilities and choose to utilize these qualities to the best of their ability. Someone with a weak sense of self-efficacy evades challenges and quickly feels discouraged by setbacks. They may not be aware of these negative reactions, and therefore do not always change their attitude. This concept is central to Bandura's social cognitive theory, "which emphasizes the role of observational learning, social experience, and reciprocal determinism in the development of personality."[28]
Developmental stages
Individuals become conscious of themselves through the development of self-awareness.[22] This particular type of self-development pertains to becoming conscious of one's own body and mental state of mind including thoughts, actions, ideas, feelings and interactions with others.[29] "Self-awareness does not occur suddenly through one particular behavior: it develops gradually through a succession of different behaviors all of which relate to the self."[30] The monitoring of one's mental states is called metacognition and it is considered to be an indicator that there is some concept of the self.[31] It is developed through an early sense of non-self components using sensory and memory sources. In developing self–awareness through self-exploration and social experiences one can broaden one's social world and become more familiar with the self.
According to Emory University's Philippe Rochat, there are five levels of self-awareness which unfold in early development and six potential prospects ranging from "Level 0" (having no self-awareness) advancing complexity to "Level 5" (explicit self-awareness).[22]
- Level 0: Confusion. At this level the individual has a degree of zero self-awareness. This person is unaware of any mirror reflection or the mirror itself. They perceive the mirror as an extension of their environment. Level 0 can also be displayed when an adult frightens himself in a mirror mistaking his own reflection as another person just for a second.
- Level 1: Differentiation. The individual realizes the mirror is able to reflect things. They see that what is in the mirror is different from what is surrounding them. At this level they can differentiate between their own movement in the mirror and the movement of the surrounding environment.
- Level 2: Situation. At this point an individual can link the movements on the mirror to what is perceived within their own body. This is the first hint of self-exploration on a projected surface where what is visualized on the mirror is special to the self.
- Level 3: Identification. This stage is characterized by the new ability to identify self: an individual can now see that what's in the mirror is not another person but actually them. It is seen when a child, instead of referring to the mirror while referring to themselves, refers to themselves while looking in the mirror.
- Level 4: Permanence. Once an individual reaches this level they can identify the self beyond the present mirror imagery. They are able to identify the self in previous pictures looking different or younger. A "permanent self" is now experienced.
- Level 5: Self-consciousness or "meta" self-awareness. At this level not only is the self seen from a first person view but it is realized that it is also seen from a third person's view. They begin to understand they can be in the mind of others. For instance, how they are seen from a public standpoint.[22]
Infancy and early childhood
It is to be kept in mind that as an infant comes into this world, they have no concept of what is around them, nor for the significance of others around them. It is throughout the first year that they gradually begin to acknowledge that their body is actually separate from that of their mother, and that they are an "active, causal agent in space". By the end of the first year, they additionally realize that their movement, as well, is separate from movement of the mother. That is a huge advance, yet they are still quite limited and cannot yet know what they look like, "in the sense that the infant cannot recognize its own face".[32] By the time an average toddler reaches 18–24 months, they will discover themselves and recognize their own reflection in the mirror,[33] however research has found that this age varies widely with differing socioeconomic levels and differences relating to culture and parenting.[34] They begin to acknowledge the fact that the image in front of them, who happens to be them, moves; indicating that they appreciate and can consider the relationship between cause and effect that is happening.[32] By the age of 24 months the toddler will observe and relate their own actions to those actions of other people and the surrounding environment.[33] Once an infant has gotten a lot of experience, and time, in front of a mirror, it is only then that they are able to recognize themselves in the reflection, and understand that it is them. For example, in a study, an experimenter took a red marker and put a fairly large red dot (so it is visible by the infant) on the infant's nose, and placed them in front of a mirror. Prior to 15 months of age, the infant will not react to this, but after 15 months of age, they will either touch their nose, wondering what it is they have on their face, or point to it. This indicates the appearance that they recognize that the image they see in the reflection of the mirror is themselves. There is somewhat of the same thing called the mirror-self recognition task, and it has been used as a research tool for numerous years, and has given, and lead to, key foundations of the infant's sense/awareness of self.[35] For example, "for Piaget, the objectification of the bodily self occurs as the infant becomes able to represent the body's spatial and causal relationship with the external world (Piaget, 1954).[36] Facial recognition places a big pivotal point in their development of self-awareness.[32] By 18 months, the infant can communicate their name to others, and upon being shown a picture they are in, they can identify themselves. By two years old, they also usually acquire gender category and age categories, saying things such as "I am a girl, not a boy" and "I am a baby or child, not a grownup". Evidently, it is not at the level of an adult or an adolescent, but as an infant moves to middle childhood and onwards to adolescence, they develop a higher level of self-awareness and self-description.[32]
As infants develop their senses, using multiple senses of in order to recognize what is around them, infants can become affected by something known as "facial multi stimulation". In one experiment by Filippetti, Farroni, and Johnson, an infant of around five months in age is given what is known as an “enfacement illusion”.[37] “Infants watched a side-by-side video display of a peer’s face being systematically stroked on the cheek with a paintbrush. During the video presentation, the infant’s own cheek was stroked in synchrony with one video and in asynchrony with the other”.[37] Infants were proven to recognize and project an image of a peer with that of their own, showing beginning signs of facial recognition cues onto one's self, with the assistance of an illusion.
Piaget
Around school age a child's awareness of personal memory transitions into a sense of one's own self. At this stage, a child begins to develop interests along with likes and dislikes. This transition enables the awareness of an individual's past, present, and future to grow as conscious experiences are remembered more often.[33] As a preschooler, they begin to give much more specific details about things, instead of generalizing. For example, the preschooler will talk about the Los Angeles Lakers basketball team, and the New York Rangers hockey team, instead of the infant just stating that he likes sports. Furthermore, they will start to express certain preferences (e.g., Tod likes mac and cheese) and will start to identify certain possessions of theirs (e.g., Lara has a bird as a pet at home). At this age, the infant is in the stage Piaget names the pre operational stage of development. The infant is very inaccurate at judging themselves because they do not have much to go about. For example, an infant at this stage will not associate that they are strong with their ability to cross the jungle gym at their school, nor will they associate the fact that they can solve a math problem with their ability to count.[32]
Adolescence
One becomes conscious of their emotions during adolescence. Most children are aware of emotions such as shame, guilt, pride and embarrassment by the age of two, but do not fully understand how those emotions affect their life.[38] By age 13, children become more in touch with these emotions and begin to apply them to their own lives. A study entitled "The Construction of the Self" found that many adolescents display happiness and self-confidence around friends, but hopelessness and anger around parents due to the fear of being a disappointment. Teenagers were also shown to feel intelligent and creative around teachers, and shy, uncomfortable and nervous around people they were not familiar with.[39]
In adolescent development, the definition self-awareness also has a more complex emotional context due to the maturity of adolescents compared to those in the early childhood phase, and these elements can include but are not limited to self-image, self-concept, and self–consciousness along many other traits that can relate to Rochat's final level of self awareness, however it is still a distinct concept within its own previous definition.[40] Social interactions mainly separate the element of self-awareness in adolescent rather than in childhood, as well as further developed emotional recognition skills in adolescents. Sandu, Pânișoară, and Pânișoară demonstrate these in their work with teenagers and demonstrates that there is a mature sense of self-awareness with students who were aged 17, which in term provides a clear structure with how elements like self-concept, self-image, and self-consciousness relate to self-awareness.[40]
Mental health
As children reach their adolescent stages of life, the acute sense of emotion has widened into a meta cognitive state in which mental health issues can become more prevalent due to their heightened emotional and social development.[41] There are elements of contextual behavioral science such as Self-as-Content, Self-as-Process and Self-as-Context, involved with adolescent self-awareness that can associate with mental health.[41] Moran, Almada, and McHugh presented the idea that these domains of self are associated with adolescent mental health in various capacities.[41] Anger management is also a domain of mental health that is associated with the concept of self-awareness in teens.[42] Self-awareness training has been linked to lowering anger management issues and reducing aggressive tendencies in adolescents “Persons having sufficient self-awareness promote relaxation and awareness about themselves and when going angry, at the first step they become aware of anger in their inside and accept it, then try to handle it”.[42]
Philosophy
Main article: Philosophy of self
Locke
An early philosophical discussion of self-awareness is that of John Locke. Locke was apparently influenced by René Descartes' statement normally translated 'I think, therefore I am' (Cogito ergo sum). In chapter XXVII "On Identity and Diversity" of Locke's An Essay Concerning Human Understanding (1689) he conceptualized consciousness as the repeated self-identification of oneself through which moral responsibility could be attributed to the subject—and therefore punishment and guiltiness justified, as critics such as Nietzsche would point out, affirming "...the psychology of conscience is not 'the voice of God in man'; it is the instinct of cruelty ... expressed, for the first time, as one of the oldest and most indispensable elements in the foundation of culture."[43][44][45] John Locke does not use the terms self-awareness or self-consciousness though.[46]
According to Locke, personal identity (the self) "depends on consciousness, not on substance".[47][failed verification] We are the same person to the extent that we are conscious of our past and future thoughts and actions in the same way as we are conscious of our present thoughts and actions. If consciousness is this "thought" which doubles all thoughts, then personal identity is only founded on the repeated act of consciousness: "This may show us wherein personal identity consists: not in the identity of substance, but ... in the identity of consciousness."[47] For example, one may claim to be a reincarnation of Plato, therefore having the same soul. However, one would be the same person as Plato only if one had the same consciousness of Plato's thoughts and actions that he himself did. Therefore, self-identity is not based on the soul. One soul may have various personalities.
Locke argues that self-identity is not founded either on the body or the substance, as the substance may change while the person remains the same. "Animal identity is preserved in identity of life, and not of substance", as the body of the animal grows and changes during its life.[47] describes a case of a prince and a cobbler in which the soul of the prince is transferred to the body of the cobbler and vice versa. The prince still views himself as a prince, though he no longer looks like one. This border-case leads to the problematic thought that since personal identity is based on consciousness, and that only oneself can be aware of his consciousness, exterior human judges may never know if they really are judging—and punishing—the same person, or simply the same body. Locke argues that one may be judged for the actions of one's body rather than one's soul, and only God knows how to correctly judge a man's actions. Men also are only responsible for the acts of which they are conscious. This forms the basis of the insanity defense which argues that one cannot be held accountable for acts in which they were unconsciously irrational, or mentally ill[48]— In reference to man's personality, Locke claims that "whatever past actions it cannot reconcile or appropriate to that present self by consciousness, it can be no more concerned in it than if they had never been done: and to receive pleasure or pain, i.e. reward or punishment, on the account of any such action, is all one as to be made happy or miserable in its first being, without any demerit at all."[49]
Disorders
The medical term for not being aware of one's deficits is anosognosia, or more commonly known as a lack of insight. Having a lack of awareness raises the risks of treatment and service nonadherence.[50] Individuals who deny having an illness may be against seeking professional help because they are convinced that nothing is wrong with them. Disorders of self-awareness frequently follow frontal lobe damage.[51] There are two common methods used to measure how severe an individual's lack of self-awareness is. The Patient Competency Rating Scale (PCRS) evaluates self-awareness in patients who have endured a traumatic brain injury.[52] PCRS is a 30-item self-report instrument which asks the subject to use a 5-point Likert scale to rate his or her degree of difficulty in a variety of tasks and functions. Independently, relatives or significant others who know the patient well are also asked to rate the patient on each of the same behavioral items. The difference between the relatives’ and patient's perceptions is considered an indirect measure of impaired self-awareness. The limitations of this experiment rest on the answers of the relatives. Results of their answers can lead to a bias. This limitation prompted a second method of testing a patient's self-awareness. Simply asking a patient why they are in the hospital or what is wrong with their body can give compelling answers as to what they see and are analyzing.[53]
Anosognosia
Anosognosia was a term coined by Joseph Babinski to describe the clinical condition in which an individual suffered from left hemiplegia following a right cerebral hemisphere stroke yet denied that there were any problems with their left arm or leg. This condition is known as anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP). This condition has evolved throughout the years and is now used to describe people who lack subjective experience in both neurological and neuropsychological cases.[54] A wide variety of disorders are associated with anosognosia. For example, patients who are blind from cortical lesions might in fact be unaware that they are blind and may state that they do not suffer from any visual disturbances. Individuals with aphasia and other cognitive disorders may also suffer from anosognosia as they are unaware of their deficiencies and when they make certain speech errors, they may not correct themselves due to their unawareness.[55] Individuals who suffer from Alzheimer's disease lack awareness; this deficiency becomes more intense throughout their disease.[56] A key issue with this disorder is that people who do have anosognosia and suffer from certain illnesses may not be aware of them, which ultimately leads them to put themselves in dangerous positions and/or environments.[55] To this day there are still no available treatments for AHP, but it has been documented that temporary remission has been used following vestibular stimulation.[57]
Dissociative identity disorder
Dissociative identity disorder or multiple personality disorder (MPD) is a disorder involving a disturbance of identity in which two or more separate and distinct personality states (or identities) control an individual's behavior at different times.[58] One identity may be different from another, and when an individual with DID is under the influence of one of their identities, they may forget their experiences when they switch to the other identity. "When under the control of one identity, a person is usually unable to remember some of the events that occurred while other personalities were in control."[59] They may experience time loss, amnesia, and adopt different mannerisms, attitudes, speech and ideas under different personalities. They are often unaware of the different lives they lead or their condition in general, feeling as though they are looking at their life through the lens of someone else, and even being unable to recognize themselves in a mirror.[60] Two cases of DID have brought awareness to the disorder, the first case being that of Eve. This patient harbored three different personalities: Eve White the good wife and mother, Eve Black the party girl, and Jane the intellectual. Under stress, her episodes would worsen. She even tried to strangle her own daughter and had no recollection of the act afterward. Eve went through years of therapy before she was able to learn how to control her alters and be mindful of her disorder and episodes. Her condition, being so rare at the time, inspired the book and film adaptation The Three Faces of Eve, as well as a memoir by Eve herself entitled I'm Eve. Doctors speculated that growing up during the Depression and witnessing horrific things being done to other people could have triggered emotional distress, periodic amnesia, and eventually DID.[61] In the second case, Shirley Mason, or Sybil, was described as having over 16 separate personalities with different characteristics and talents. Her accounts of horrific and sadistic abuse by her mother during childhood prompted doctors to believe that this trauma caused her personalities to split, furthering the unproven idea that this disorder was rooted in child abuse, while also making the disorder famous. In 1998 however, Sybil's case was exposed as a sham. Her therapist would encourage Sybil to act as her other alter ego although she felt perfectly like herself. Her condition was exaggerated in order to seal book deals and television adaptations.[62] Awareness of this disorder began to crumble shortly after this finding. To this day, no proven cause of DID has been found, but treatments such as psychotherapy, medications, hypnotherapy, and adjunctive therapies have proven to be very effective.[63]
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a range of neurodevelopmental disabilities that can adversely impact social communication and create behavioral challenges (Understanding Autism, 2003).[64] "Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and autism are both general terms for a group of complex disorders of brain development. These disorders are characterized, in varying degrees, by difficulties in social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication and repetitive behaviors."[65] ASDs can also cause imaginative abnormalities and can range from mild to severe, especially in sensory-motor, perceptual and affective dimensions.[66] Children with ASD may struggle with self-awareness and self acceptance. Their different thinking patterns and brain processing functions in the area of social thinking and actions may compromise their ability to understand themselves and social connections to others.[67] About 75% diagnosed autistics are mentally handicapped in some general way and the other 25% diagnosed with Asperger's Syndrome show average to good cognitive functioning.[68] When we compare our own behavior to the morals and values that we were taught, we can focus more attention on ourselves which increases self-awareness. To understand the many effects of autism spectrum disorders on those afflicted have led many scientists to theorize what level of self-awareness occurs and in what degree. Research found that ASD can be associated with intellectual disability and difficulties in motor coordination and attention. It can also result in physical health issues as well, such as sleep and gastrointestinal disturbances. As a result of all those problems, individuals are literally unaware of themselves.[69] It is well known that children suffering from varying degrees of autism struggle in social situations. Scientists at the University of Cambridge have produced evidence that self-awareness is a main problem for people with ASD. Researchers used functional magnetic resonance scans (FMRI) to measure brain activity in volunteers being asked to make judgments about their own thoughts, opinions, preferences, as well as about someone else's. One area of the brain closely examined was the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vMPFC) which is known to be active when people think about themselves.[70]
A study out of Stanford University has tried to map out brain circuits with understanding self-awareness in Autism Spectrum Disorders.[71] This study suggests that self-awareness is primarily lacking in social situations but when in private they are more self-aware and present. It is in the company of others while engaging in interpersonal interaction that the self-awareness mechanism seems to fail. Higher functioning individuals on the ASD scale have reported that they are more self-aware when alone unless they are in sensory overload or immediately following social exposure.[72] Self-awareness dissipates when an autistic is faced with a demanding social situation. This theory suggests that this happens due to the behavioral inhibitory system which is responsible for self-preservation. This is the system that prevents human from self-harm like jumping out of a speeding bus or putting our hand on a hot stove. Once a dangerous situation is perceived then the behavioral inhibitory system kicks in and restrains our activities. "For individuals with ASD, this inhibitory mechanism is so powerful, it operates on the least possible trigger and shows an over sensitivity to impending danger and possible threats.[72] Some of these dangers may be perceived as being in the presence of strangers, or a loud noise from a radio. In these situations self-awareness can be compromised due to the desire of self preservation, which trumps social composure and proper interaction.
The Hobson hypothesis reports that autism begins in infancy due to the lack of cognitive and linguistic engagement which in turn results in impaired reflective self-awareness. In this study ten children with Asperger's Syndrome were examined using the Self-understanding Interview. This interview was created by Damon and Hart and focuses on seven core areas or schemas that measure the capacity to think in increasingly difficult levels. This interview will estimate the level of self understanding present. "The study showed that the Asperger group demonstrated impairment in the 'self-as-object' and 'self-as-subject' domains of the Self-understanding Interview, which supported Hobson's concept of an impaired capacity for self-awareness and self-reflection in people with ASD.".[73] Self-understanding is a self description in an individual's past, present and future. Without self-understanding it is reported that self-awareness is lacking in people with ASD.
Joint attention (JA) was developed as a teaching strategy to help increase positive self-awareness in those with autism spectrum disorder.[74] JA strategies were first used to directly teach about reflected mirror images and how they relate to their reflected image. Mirror Self Awareness Development (MSAD) activities were used as a four-step framework to measure increases in self-awareness in those with ASD. Self-awareness and knowledge is not something that can simply be taught through direct instruction. Instead, students acquire this knowledge by interacting with their environment.[74] Mirror understanding and its relation to the development of self leads to measurable increases in self-awareness in those with ASD. It also proves to be a highly engaging and highly preferred tool in understanding the developmental stages of self- awareness.
There have been many different theories and studies done on what degree of self-awareness is displayed among people with autism spectrum disorder. Scientists have done research about the various parts of the brain associated with understanding self and self-awareness. Studies have shown evidence of areas of the brain that are impacted by ASD. Other theories suggest that helping an individual learn more about themselves through Joint Activities, such as the Mirror Self Awareness Development may help teach positive self-awareness and growth. In helping to build self-awareness it is also possible to build self-esteem and self acceptance. This in turn can help to allow the individual with ASD to relate better to their environment and have better social interactions with others.
Schizophrenia
Schizophrenia is a chronic psychiatric illness characterized by excessive dopamine activity in the mesolimbic tract and insufficient dopamine activity in the mesocortical tract leading to symptoms of psychosis along with poor cognition in socialization. Under the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, people with schizophrenia have a combination of positive, negative and psychomotor symptoms. These cognitive disturbances involve rare beliefs and/or thoughts of a distorted reality that creates an abnormal pattern of functioning for the patient. The cause of schizophrenia has a substantial genetic component involving many genes. While the heritability of schizophrenia has been found to be around 80%, only about 60% of sufferers report a positive family history of the disorder, and ultimately the cause is thought to be a combination of genetic and environmental factors.[75] It is believed that the experience of stressful life events is an environmental factor that can trigger the onset of schizophrenia in individuals who already are at risk from genetics and age.[76] The level of self-awareness among patients with schizophrenia is a heavily studied topic.
Schizophrenia as a disease state is characterized by severe cognitive dysfunction and it is uncertain to what extent patients are aware of this deficiency. Medalia and Lim (2004) [77] investigated patients’ awareness of their cognitive deficit in the areas of attention, nonverbal memory, and verbal memory. Results from this study (N=185) revealed large discrepancy in patients’ assessment of their cognitive functioning relative to the assessment of their clinicians. Though it is impossible to access one's consciousness and truly understand what a schizophrenic believes, regardless in this study, patients were not aware of their cognitive dysfunctional reasoning. In the DSM-5, to receive a diagnosis of schizophrenia, they must have two or more of the following symptoms in the duration of one month: delusions*, hallucinations*, disorganized speech*, grossly disorganized/catatonic behavior and negative symptoms (*these three symptoms above all other symptoms must be present to correctly diagnose a patient.) Sometimes these symptoms are very prominent and are treated with a combination of antipsychotics (i.e. haloperidol, loxapine), atypical antipsychotics (such as clozapine and risperdone) and psychosocial therapies that include family interventions and socials skills. When a patient is undergoing treatment and recovering from the disorder, the memory of their behavior is present in a diminutive amount; thus, self-awareness of diagnoses of schizophrenia after treatment is rare, as well as subsequent to onset and prevalence in the patient.
The above findings are further supported by a study conducted by Amador and colleagues.[78] The study suggests a correlation exists between patient insight, compliance, and disease progression. Investigators assess insight of illness was assessed via Scale to Assess Unawareness of Mental Disorder and was used along with rating of psychopathology, course of illness, and compliance with treatments in a sample of 43 patients. Patients with poor insight are less likely to be compliant with treatment and are more likely to have a poorer prognosis. Patients with hallucinations sometimes experience positive symptoms, which can include delusions of reference, thought insertion/withdrawal, thought broadcast, delusions of persecution, grandiosity, and many more. These psychoses skew the patient's perspectives of reality in ways in which they truly believe are really happening. For instance, a patient that is experiencing delusions of reference may believe while watching the weather forecast that when the weatherman says it will rain, he is really sending a message to the patient in which rain symbolizes a specific warning completely irrelevant to what the weather is. Another example would be thought broadcast, which is when a patient believes that everyone can hear their thoughts. These positive symptoms sometimes are so severe to where the schizophrenic believes that something is crawling on them or smelling something that is not there in reality. These strong hallucinations are intense and difficult to convince the patient that they do not exist outside of their cognitive beliefs, making it extremely difficult for a patient to understand and become self-aware that what they are experiencing is in fact not there.
Furthermore, a study by Bedford and Davis[79] (2013) was conducted to look at the association of denial vs. acceptance of multiple facets of schizophrenia (self-reflection, self-perception, and insight) and its effect on self-reflection (N=26). Study results suggest patients with increased disease denial have lower recollection for self-evaluated mental illnesses. To a great extent, disease denial creates a hardship for patients to undergo recovery because their feelings and sensations are intensely outstanding. But just as this and the above studies imply, a large proportion of schizophrenics do not have self-awareness of their illness for many factors and severity of reasoning of their diagnoses.
Bipolar disorder
Bipolar disorder is an illness that causes shifts in mood, energy, and ability to function. Self-awareness is crucial in those suffering from this disease, as they must be able to distinguish between feeling a certain way because of the disorder or because of separate issues. "Personality, behavior, and dysfunction affect your bipolar disorder, so you must 'know' yourself in order to make the distinction."[80] This disorder is a difficult one to diagnose, as self-awareness changes with mood. "For instance, what might appear to you as confidence and clever ideas for a new business venture might be a pattern of grandiose thinking and manic behavior".[81] Issues occur between understanding irrationality in a mood swing and being completely wrapped in a manic episode, rationalizing that the exhibited behaviors are normal.
It is important to be able to distinguish what are symptoms of bipolar disorder and what is not. A study done by Mathew et al. was done with the aim of "examining the perceptions of illness in self and among other patients with bipolar disorder in remission".[82]
The study took place at the Department of Psychiatry, Christian Medical College, Vellore, India, which is a centre that specializes in the "management of patients with mental and behavioural disorders".[82] Eighty two patients (thirty two female and fifty male) agreed to partake in the study. These patients met the "International Classification of Diseases – 10 diagnostic criteria for a diagnosis of bipolar disorder I or II and were in remission"[82] and were put through a variety of baseline assessments before beginning the study. These baseline assessments included using a vignette, which was then used as an assessment tool during their follow-up. Patients were then randomly divided into two groups, one who would be following a "structured educational intervention programme" [82](experimental group), while the other would be following "usual care" (control group).
The study was based on an interview in which patients were asked an array of open-eded questions regarding topics such as "perceived causes, consequences, severity and its effects on body, emotion, social network and home life, and on work, severity, possible course of action, help-seeking behaviour and the role of the doctor/healer".[82] The McNemar test was then used to compare the patients perspective of the illness versus their explanation of the illness. The results of the study show that the beliefs that patients associated with their illness corresponds with the possible causes of the disorder,[82] whereas "studies done among patients during periods of active psychosis have recorded disagreement between their assessments of their own illness".[83] This ties in to how difficult self-awareness is within people who suffer from bipolar disorder.
Although this study was done on a population that were in remission from the disease, the distinction between patients during "active psychosis" versus those in remission shows the evolution of their self-awareness throughout their journey to recovery.
Plants
Self-discrimination in plants is found within their roots, tendrils and flowers that avoid themselves but not others in their environment.[84]
Self-incompatibility mechanism providing evidence for self-awareness in plants
Self-awareness in plants is a fringe topic in the field of self-awareness, and is researched predominantly by botanists. The claim that plants are capable of perceiving self lies in the evidence found that plants will not reproduce with themselves due to a gene selecting mechanism. In addition, vining plants have been shown to avoid coiling around themselves, due to chemical receptors in the plants' tendrils. Unique to plants, awareness of self means that the plant can recognise self, whereas all other known conceptions of self-awareness is the ability to recognise what is not self.
Recognition and rejection of self in plant reproduction
Research by June B. Nasrallah discovered that the plant's pollination mechanism also serves as a mechanism against self-reproduction, which lays out the foundation of scientific evidence that plants could be considered as self-aware organisms. The SI (Self-incompatibility) mechanism in plants is unique in the sense that awareness of self derives from the capacity to recognise self, rather than non-self. The SI mechanism function depends primarily on the interaction between genes S-locus receptor protein kinase (SRK) and S-locus cysteine-rich protein gene (SCR). In cases of self-pollination, SRK and SCR bind to activate SKR, Inhibiting pollen from fertilizing. In cases of cross-pollination, SRK and SCR do not bind and therefore SRK is not activated, causing the pollen to fertilise. In simple terms, the receptors either accept or reject the genes present in the pollen, and when the genes are from the same plant, the SI mechanism described above creates a reaction to prevent the pollen from fertilising.
Self-discrimination in the tendrils of the vine Cayratia japonica mediated by physiological connection
The research by Yuya Fukano and Akira Yamawo provides a link between self-discrimination in vining plants and amongst other classifications where the mechanism discovery has already been established. It also contributes to the general foundation of evidence of self-discrimination mechanisms in plants. The article makes the claim that the biological self-discrimination mechanism that is present in both flowering plants and ascidians, are also present in vining plants. They tested this hypothesis by doing touch tests with self neighbouring and non-self neighbouring pairs of plants. the test was performed by placing the sets of plants close enough for their tendrils to interact with one-another. Evidence of self-discrimination in above-ground plants is demonstrated in the results of the touch testing, which showed that in cases of connected self plants, severed self plants and non-self plants, the rate of tendril activity and likeliness to coil was higher among separated plants than those attached via rhizomes.
Theater
Theater also concerns itself with other awareness besides self-awareness. There is a possible correlation between the experience of the theater audience and individual self-awareness. As actors and audiences must not "break" the fourth wall in order to maintain context, so individuals must not be aware of the artificial, or the constructed perception of his or her reality. This suggests that both self-awareness and the social constructs applied to others are artificial continuums just as theater is. Theatrical efforts such as Six Characters in Search of an Author, or The Wonderful Wizard of Oz, construct yet another layer of the fourth wall, but they do not destroy the primary illusion. Refer to Erving Goffman's Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience.[citation needed]
Science fiction]
In science fiction, self-awareness describes an essential human property that often (depending on the circumstances of the story) bestows personhood onto a non-human. If a computer, alien or other object is described as "self-aware", the reader may assume that it will be treated as a completely human character, with similar rights, capabilities and desires to a normal human being.[85] The words "sentience", "sapience" and "consciousness" are used in similar ways in science fiction.
Robotics
In order to be "self-aware," robots can use internal models to simulate their own actions.[86]
See also
- Animal consciousness
- Bicameralism (psychology)
- Boltzmann brain
- Cartesian theater
- Childhood amnesia
- Confidence
- Deindividuation
- Dunning–Kruger effect
- Feldenkrais Method
- Higher consciousness
- Human self-reflection
- Intentionality
- Insight § Psychiatry
- Lucid dreaming
- Memory inhibition
- Mental noting
- Mindfulness
- Mirror test
- Modesty
- Outline of self
- Psychological mindedness
- Robert Langs
- Self-consciousness
- Self-knowledge (psychology)
- Situation awareness
- Vedanta
- Vipassanā
- Yoga nidra
References
- ^ Google def: Self-awareness is the ability to focus on yourself and how your actions, thoughts, or emotions do or don't align with your internal standards. If you're highly self-aware, you can objectively evaluate yourself, manage your emotions, align your behavior with your values, and understand correctly how others perceive you.May 11, 2018
- ^ "Self-awareness - Definition of Self-awareness by Merriam-Webster".
- ^ Jabr, Ferris (2012). "Self-Awareness with a Simple Brain". Scientific American Mind. 23 (5): 28–29. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind1112-28.
- ^ "What Self-Awareness Really Is (and How to Cultivate It)". Harvard Business Review. January 4, 2018. Retrieved March 6, 2018.
- ^ Oberman, L.; Ramachandran, V.S. (2009). "Reflections on the Mirror Neuron System: Their Evolutionary Functions Beyond Motor Representation". In Pineda, J.A. (ed.). Mirror Neuron Systems: The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition. Humana Press. pp. 39–62. ISBN 978-1-934115-34-3.
- ^ Ramachandran, V.S. (January 1, 2009). "Self Awareness: The Last Frontier, Edge Foundation web essay". Retrieved July 26, 2011.
- ^ . PMC 2680990. Missing or empty
|title=
(help) - ^ Body Awareness in Anorexia Nervosa: Disturbances in "Body Image" and "Satiety"
- ^ ab Moore, Chris; Mealiea, Jennifer; Garon, Nancy; Povinelli, Daniel J. (March 1, 2007). "The Development of Body Self-Awareness". Infancy. 11(2): 157–174. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220.x. ISSN 1532-7078.
- ^ Brownell, Celia A.; Zerwas, Stephanie; Ramani, Geetha B. (September 1, 2007). ""So Big": The Development of Body Self-Awareness in Toddlers". Child Development. 78 (5): 1426–1440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01075.x. ISSN 1467-8624. PMC 3351035. PMID 17883440.
- ^ Plotnik, Joshua; Waal, Frans; Reiss, Diana (2006). "Self recognition in an Asian elephant". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 103 (45): 17053–57. Bibcode:2006PNAS..10317053P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0608062103. PMC 1636577. PMID 17075063.
- ^ "Self-awareness in animals" (PDF).
- ^ Bekoff, M (2002). "Animal reflections". Nature. 419 (6904): 255. doi:10.1038/419255a. PMID 12239547.
- ^ Gallup Jr, GG; Anderson, JR; Shillito, DJ (2002). "The mirror test. The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on October 6, 2014.
- ^ Plotnik et al., (2006)
- ^ ab c d e f g Prior, H; Schwarz, A; Güntürkün, O (2008). "Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition". PLOS Biology. 6 (8): e202. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202. PMC 2517622. PMID 18715117.
- ^ Tennesen, M (2003). "Do Dolphins Have a Sense of Self?". National Wildlife. World Edition.
- ^ Bard, Kim (2006). "Self-Awareness in Human and Chimpanzee Infants: What Is Measured and What Is Meant by the Mark and Mirror Test?". Infancy. 9 (2): 191–219. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0902_6.
- ^ Alison, Motluk. "Mirror test shows magpies aren't so bird-brained". New Scientist. Retrieved November 26, 2014.
- ^ How the Body Shapes the Way We Think: A New View of Intelligence, Rolf Pfeifer, Josh Bongard October 2006
- ^ The Unpredictable Species: What Makes Humans Unique, Lieberman, P 2013
- ^ ab c d Rochat, Philippe (2003). "Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life" (PDF). Consciousness and Cognition. 12 (4): 717–731. doi:10.1016/s1053-8100(03)00081-3. PMID 14656513.
- ^ Duval, Shelley and Wicklund, Robert A (1972) A theory of objective self awareness Academic Press.
- ^ Anthony P. Cohen, Self-Consciousness: An Alternative Anthropology of Identity, 1994. Google Books link.
- ^ Thomas S. Duval Self-Awareness and Causal Attribution, p. 1, Springer, 2001 ISBN 978-0-7923-7501-2
- ^ Demetriou, A., & Kazi, S. (2001). Unity and modularity in the mind and the self: Studies on the relationships between self-awareness, personality, and intellectual development from childhood to adolescence. London: Routledge.
- ^ Demetriou, A.; Kazi, S. (2006). "Self-awareness in g (with processing efficiency and reasoning)". Intelligence. 34 (3): 297–317. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.10.002.
- ^ Cherry, Kendra. "What is Self-efficacy?". psychology.about.com.
- ^ Geangu, E. (2008). "Notes on Self-Awareness Development in Early Infancy" (PDF). Gognitie, Creier, Comportamente. 12 (1).
- ^ Bertenthal, B.; Fischer, K. W. (1978). "Development of Self-Recognition in the Infant" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 14 (1): 44–50. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.550.1903. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.14.1.44.
- ^ Justin.J, Couchman (2014). "Humans and monkeys distinguish between self-generated, opposing, and random actions". Animal Cognition. 18 (1): 231–238. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0792-6. PMC 4282946. PMID 25108418.
- ^ ab c d e Thomas D. Yawkey Z; James E. Johnson (eds.). Integrative processes and socialization early to middle childhood. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 9780203767696.
- ^ ab c Rochat, Philippe (1998). "Self-Perception and Action in Infancy"(PDF). Exp Brain Res. 123 (1–2): 102–109. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.602.6987. doi:10.1007/s002210050550. PMID 9835398.
- ^ Lynn Broesch, Tanya (September 2010). "Cultural Variations in Children's Mirror Self-Recognition" (PDF). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology.
- ^ Moore, Maeliea, Garon, Chris, Jennifer, Nancy (2007). "The Development of Body Self-Awareness". 11. 11 (2): 157–174. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220.x.
- ^ Moore, Maeliea, Garon, Chris, Jennifer, Nancy (2007). "The Development of Body Self-Awareness". Infancy. 11 (2): 157–174. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220.x.
- ^ ab Filippetti, M.; Farroni, T.; Johnson, M.H. (2016). "Five-Month-old Infants' Discrimination of Visual–Tactile Synchronous Facial Stimulation"(PDF). Infant and Child Development. 25 (3): 317–322. doi:10.1002/icd.1977 – via EBSCO.
- ^ Zeanah, Charles (2009). Handbook of Infant Mental Health. New York: Guilford Press.
- ^ Harter, Susan (1999). The Construction of the Self. Guilford Press.
- ^ ab Sandu, Cristina Marina; Pânişoarã, Georgeta; Pânişoarã, Ion Ovidiu (May 5, 2015). "Study on the Development of Self-awareness in Teenagers". Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences. 180: 1656–1660. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.05.060. ISSN 1877-0428.
- ^ ab c Moran, Orla; Almada, Priscilla; McHugh, Louise (January 1, 2018). "An investigation into the relationship between the three selves (Self-as-Content, Self-as-Process and Self-as-Context) and mental health in adolescents". Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science. 7: 55–62. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.01.002. ISSN 2212-1447.
- ^ ab Mohammadiarya, Alireza; Sarabi, Salar Dousti; Shirazi, Mahmoud; Lachinani, Fatemeh; Roustaei, Amin; Abbasi, Zohre; Ghasemzadeh, Azizreza (January 1, 2012). "The Effect of Training Self-Awareness and Anger Management on Aggression Level in Iranian Middle School Students". Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences. 46: 987–991. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.05.235. ISSN 1877-0428.
- ^ Ecce Homo by Friedrich Wilhelm Nietzsche p. 117
- ^ Richard Schacht Nietzsche, Genealogy, Morality, p. 244, University of California Press, 1994 ISBN 978-0-520-08318-9
- ^ What Nietzsche Taught pp. 209–10.
- ^ Locke, John (1775). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding by John Locke. J. Beecroft. Retrieved February 11, 2012.
John Locke Concerning Human Understand.
- ^ ab c Kendra, Cherry. "What Is Self-Awareness?". Retrieved November 25, 2014.
- ^ Abraham S. Goldstein The Insanity Defense, p. 9, Yale University Press, 1967 ISBN 978-0-300-00099-3
- ^ Kendra, Cherry. "What Is Self-Awareness?". Retrieved November 26,2014.
- ^ Xavier, Amador. "Anosognosia (Lack of Insight) Fact Sheet". Retrieved November 26, 2014.
- ^ Prigatano, George P. "Disorders of Behavior and Self-Awareness". thebarrow.org.
- ^ Leathem, Janet M.; Murphy, Latesha J.; Flett, Ross A. (August 9, 2010). "Self- and Informant-Ratings on the Patient Competency Rating Scale in Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 20 (5): 694–705. doi:10.1076/jcen.20.5.694.1122. PMID 10079045.
- ^ George P, Prigatano, PhD. "Impaired Awareness, Finger Tapping, and Rehabilitation Outcome After Brain Injury". Retrieved November 26,2014.
- ^ Prigatano, George P (December 1, 2009). "Anosognosia: clinical and ethical considerations". Current Opinion in Neurology. 22 (6): 606–611. doi:10.1097/wco.0b013e328332a1e7. ISSN 1350-7540. PMID 19809315.
- ^ ab Heilman, K. M.; Barrett, A. M.; Adair, J. C. (November 29, 1998). "Possible mechanisms of anosognosia: a defect in self–awareness". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 353 (1377): 1903–1909. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0342. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 1692420. PMID 9854262.
- ^ Sack, M.; Cassidy, J. T.; Bole, G. G. (December 1975). "Prognostic factors in polyarteritis". The Journal of Rheumatology. 2 (4): 411–420. ISSN 0315-162X. PMID 1533.
- ^ Rubens, A. B. (July 1985). "Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect". Neurology. 35 (7): 1019–1024. doi:10.1212/WNL.35.7.1019. ISSN 0028-3878. PMID 4010940.
- ^ National alliance on mental illness. "Mental illnesses". Retrieved November 27, 2014.
- ^ "Dissociative Disorders". National Alliance on Mental Illness.
- ^ Dryden-Edwards, Roxanne. Conrad Stöppler, Melissa (ed.). "Dissociative Identity Disorder Symptoms and Treatments". MedicineNet. Retrieved October 16, 2017.
- ^ Ramsland, Katherine. "Multiple Personalities: Crime and Defense". crimelibrary.com.
- ^ Ramsland, Katherine. "Multiple Personalities: Crime and Defense". crimelibrary.com.
- ^ Dryden-Edwards, Roxanne. "Dissociative Identity Disorder". medicinenet.com.
- ^ National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. "Autism Fact Sheet". Office of Communications and Public Liaison. Retrieved November 27, 2014.
- ^ "Autistic disorder".
- ^ "Recognizing Autism Spectrum Disorder". autism.org.
- ^ Sarita, Freedman, PhD. "Self-Awareness and Self-Acceptance in School-Age Students with ASD". 2014 Autism Asperger's Digest. AADigest is a division of Future Horizons. Retrieved November 27, 2014.
- ^ Victoria, McGeer (2004). "Autistic Self-Awareness". Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology. 11 (September, 2004): 235–251. doi:10.1353/ppp.2004.0066.
- ^ Autism, Speaks. "What Is Autism? What is Autism Spectrum Disorder?". autismspeaks.org/what-autism. Retrieved November 26,2014.
- ^ "How the autistic brain distinguishes oneself from others". Science Daily. December 14, 2009.
- ^ Uddin, Lucina (October 29, 2008). "Neural Basis of Self and Other Representation in Autism: An fMRI Study of Self-Face Recognition". PLOS ONE. 3 (10): e3526. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.3526U. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003526. PMC 2568959. PMID 18958161.
- ^ ab Uddin, LQ (June 2011). "The self in autism: an emerging view from neuroimaging". Neurocase. 17 (3): 201–8. doi:10.1080/13554794.2010.509320. PMC 3117464. PMID 21207316.
- ^ Jackson, P; Skirrow, P; Hare DJ (May 2012). "Asperger through the looking glass: an exploratory study of self-understanding in people with Asperger's syndrome". J Autism Dev Disord. 42 (5): 697–706. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1296-8. PMID 21647793.
- ^ ab "University of Pennsylvania".
- ^ O'Donovan MC, Williams NM, Owen MJ (October 2003). "Recent advances in the genetics of schizophrenia". Human Molecular Genetics. 12 Spec No 2: R125–33. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddg302. PMID 12952866.
- ^ Day, R. (August 23, 2007). "Life events and schizophrenia: The "triggering" hypothesis". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 64 (2): 97–122. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1981.tb00765.x. PMID 7032227.
- ^ Medalia, Amelia (2004). "Self-awareness of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research. 71 (2–3): 331–338. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.003. PMID 15474903.
- ^ Amador; Lim (1993). "Assessment of insight in psychosis". American Journal of Psychiatry. 150 (6): 873–879. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.457.5515. doi:10.1176/ajp.150.6.873. PMID 8494061.
- ^ Bedford; Davis (2013). "Denial of illness in schizophrenia as a disturbance of self-reflection, self-perception and insight". Schizophrenia Research. 152 (1): 89–96. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.006. PMID 23890596.
- ^ "Self Awareness". Bipolar Insights.
- ^ Tartakovsky, Margarita (May 17, 2016). "Living with Bipolar Disorder". psychcentral.com.
- ^ ab c d e f Mathew, Anandit J.; Samuel, Beulah; Jacob, K.S. (2009). "Perceptions of Illness in Self and in Others Among Patients With Bipolar Disorder". International Journal of Social Psychiatry. 56 (5): 462–470. doi:10.1177/0020764009106621. PMID 19651694.
- ^ McEvoy, J. P.; Schooler, N. R.; Friedman, E.; Steingard, S.; Allen, M. (1993). "Use of psychopathology vignettes by patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and by mental health professionals to judge patients' insight". American Journal of Psychiatry. 150 (11): 1649–1653. doi:10.1176/ajp.150.11.1649. PMID 8214172.
- ^ Fukano, Yuya; Yamawo, Akira (August 26, 2015). "Self-discrimination in the tendrils of the vine is mediated by physiological connection". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 282 (1814): 20151379. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1379. PMC 4571702. PMID 26311669.
- ^ Robert Kolker Stanley Kubrick's 2001: A Space Odyssey, p. 106, Oxford University Press US, 2006 ISBN 978-0-19-517452-6
- ^ Winfield, Alan FT. "Robots with internal models: a route to self-aware and hence safer robots." (2014): 237-252.
External links[edit]
Wikiquote has quotations related to: Self-awareness |
- Ashley GC, Reiter-Palmon R Self-Awareness and the Evolution of Leaders: The Need for a Better Measure of Self-Awareness September 2012
- 3-Step Process for Practicing Self-Awareness, Stephen Warley, Life Skills That Matter, March 2016
No comments:
Post a Comment