Wednesday, March 22, 2023

The Origins of Religion: The God of Creation





SUGGESTED REFERENCE
A free online textbook for Historical Geology courses


* * * * * * * * *


The Origins of Religion:
The God of Creation


As we continue to examine the origins of religion I wish to make a quick apology to my Christian friends and readers who would prefer a simpler reading of the bible. At one time I had as well but no longer.

You should know that Adam and Eve did not drop out of the sky already pre-formed and living in an idyllic garden with a talking snake. That the Tower of Babel was not the start of many languages. And that Noah's Flood was neither a global event nor singular incident.

And most of all, the stories in Genesis do have a very deep and rich history but not one which goes back eons-and-eons into man's primative eras. That Genesis' oral teachings (say, 2400 BC) and written pages (say, 750-350 BC) were speaking only of their most recent memory passed to them from earlier civilizations.

Thus I have written this extended series of articles on the evolution of man and religion as a necessity which much of the church has tended to ignore.

Also...

I think I need to spend some time in this post by re-orienting our Christian approach to God's formation of creation by asking the question, "What Does It Mean When We Say that God is Creator?"

This statement will require addressing some deeply held Christian beliefs which must be adjusted when re-assessing modern Christianity's ideas of "God and creation."

Which means my next post will be better able to list out and evaluate the underlying commonalities we might expect to find when thinking through the origins, or evolution, of religion - irrespective its many evolutionary-specific eras, portrayals, comportments, beliefs, and shared communions.


What Do We Mean When We Say
"God is Creator"?

Here is my tentative list of things I mean when I say God is my Creator.... This is not meant to be a comprehensive list but rather as way for establishing discussion between one another.

  • First, the origin of religion does not begin with doctrine. The academic development of a system of theological doctrine is a new form of believing in God. A substitute form of faith. More like a faith in a faith rather than as a faith in God who is described by doctrine.
  • Church theology in its systems, creeds, philosophies, religious rites, and behaviors are more of a modern day occurrence which has been developing over the past 2000 years of the Church Age. Before the Church we might say there were very few, if any, "systematized" collections of belief in God before the Church Fathers:
Wikipedia - The Church Fathers, Early Church Fathers, Christian Fathers, or Fathers of the Church were ancient and influential Christian theologians and writers who established the intellectual and doctrinal foundations of Christianity. The historical period in which they worked became known as the Patristic Era and spans approximately from the late 1st to mid-8th centuries, flourishing in particular during the 4th and 5th centuries, when Christianity was in the process of establishing itself as the state church of the Roman Empire.

In traditional dogmatic theology, authors considered Church Fathers are treated as authoritative, and a somewhat restrictive definition is used. The academic field of patristics, the study of the Church Fathers, has extended the scope of the term, and there is no definitive list. Some, such as Origen and Tertullian, made major contributions to the development of later Christian theology, but certain elements of their teaching were then later condemned [in later church eras].
  • Next, much of the religious behaviors and rites of homo sapiens (c.150,000 BCE and forwards) most likely had a "designated" shaman who plead before the gods of storm and wind, life and death, for the clan or tribe.
  • Further, a form or type of shamanism is as old as man's line of homonids (approx. 2,500,000 million years ago); and if taking into account our apian relatives, then the origins of religion is as old as 25,000,000 years ago. (Yes, I realize this is shocking as we don't normally think about such things as modern-day Christians).
  • And since the origins of religion - or its evolutionary antecendents - go so very far back one must assume that the "spark of divinity" - or the "fellowship of creation" with it's Creator-Redeemer God - goes alllllll the way back as well.... How far? All the way back to creation's very beginning. So what then would this mean or imply?
  • That God's loving Self to abide, indwell, be alongside, and be present momentarily and eternally with creation began at the beginning. And will so abide faithfully, fully, essentially, fundamentally, and deeply to creation's eternal, infinite end. So great is the love of God.
  • Meaning, God isn't the Father of creation if God were to leave us on our own. Nor would Jesus or the Apostle's teaching be correct when they each said "God is with us... ALWAYS".
  • More simply, we may assuredly know that the presence of God in creation, or the divinity of God held within creation's very DNA, "goes all the way down (to the merest atom) and all the up to up" (to the highest evolutionary strain... (which temporarily points to us). There is no part of Creation which God has abandoned. Creation IS (or, we are) because God IS.
  • Hence, we may surmise via metaphysical process theology that as God is relational, experiential, panpsychic, and characterized by love, that so is God's creation panrelational, panexperiential, and panpsychic... (the love part we'll discuss in a moment).
  • And although systematized classic theology can say these things it is too often at odds with itself as it views God as far away and beyond us in God's transcendency over creation because God is Holy. That God's holiness "sets-God-apart-from" creation. Which is untrue.
  • It is however more true to say that God is Other-than creation but not that God is un-present with creation. It is a logical fallacy based upon a unbiblical theodicy confusing divine holiness with sin and judgment.
  • It is my view that God's divine sovereignty must be redefined in terms of divine love and immanency as I have discussed at length over the years when exploring and dissecting Whiteheadian process philosophy. That the ideas of man attributing to God divine wrath and anger, transcendent separateness and abandonment from creation are flat out wrong. The prophet's may have preached it to Israel but we misunderstand it incorrectly when judging God to have left us when it was we, ourselves, who left God. 
  • This God of Israel, and of the bible, is love. And love is not any of these things. God is unlike the pagan gods, the Greek gods, or our idolatrous beliefs about God. God abides eternally and presently with us at all times in love... NOT wrath and judgment nor abandonment. God is not dipolar. God is One.
  • Moreover, Whitehead described process philosophy as the Philosophy of Organism. That creation is not a metaphysically reductive, mechanical, or inanimate object (ala Plato et al) but that creation - including a proper processually organic metaphysic to go with it - is a living, dynamic, ever-changing, and thus processually Divine Organism - of which WE are a living part of it connected through and through and through.
  • Why? Because divinity goes all the way down and all the way up. There is nowhere in creation which is not beheld in God.
  • Which means that classical church theology and creedal structures are built on non-processual philosophical ideas. As such, yesteryear's church doctrines and beliefs about God are incomplete, misleading, and unhelpful.
  • Further, a process-based theology built on God's Love says that God's sovereignty is always defined in terms of divine love and not power... nor, as mentioned earlier, by divine abandonment or separation.
  • That God's LOVING sovereignty is the foundation on which all of God's attributes are founded, defined, operate, or behave. Sovereignty for many bespeak divine power; but for a process theologian it refers to God's loving presence and eterternal / intimate relationality with us and creation.
  • That obscure, off-putting words like "sacred, mystical, or sin" take on new meanings beyond what they should mean in church parlance. Referring to unloving abandonment or divine wrath. Idolatrous words more describing man than accurately describing God.
  • Moreover, all creation is beloved and endowed with God's divinity... if not imaged in God's very Self throughout its parts, processes, yearnings, and angsts.
  • Process theology therefore takes commonplace church words like transcendence to mean that God is Other-than Creation but NEVER separate from creation
  • Process theology also takes other words like divine omnipresence to say that God is ever-and-always Immanently with and within (in some sense) to creation. That God abidesbecause of God's Love which the Apostle Paul says "we can never be separated from". Not even by the bad doctrinal words and preachings of the church building on the intemperate sands of man's love rather than on the rocky bastions of God's love..
  • And sin? Will that's a theodicy problem pertaining to the subject of good and evil. Statedly, a loving process theology avows unequivocally that God's Self was poured into creation at it's beginning. That with God's love came (indeterminate) freewill as part-and-parcel of the  divine Self.
  • Why? Because God is a dynamic, processual Being-ever-Becoming in God's experience with creation which is, itself, like to it's God, ever becoming. In Ex 3.14, God says of God's Self, "I AM WHO I AM. Meaning, "I am that which I am becoming." As creation evolves so does God... not as ontologially incomplete but in relational fullness meeting every evolutionary moment with God's relationally becoming Self. God is there as God will be and must be.
  • And because God is a God who can meet the evolving presence of the moment with God's own becoming Self  then we may correctly say that all things are processual in their nature even as god is in God's own essence. Like father, like son or daughter.
  • That creation's evolutionary character from science, to cosmology, to metaphysics, and even the very ontology of its being, is fully and completely a singular, processually evolving, Organism from moment to procesdual  moment.
  • Whether of God or the created, Beingness is NEVER static. It is always Evolving. Becoming. Concrescing. Why? Because all things are always in living relationships with all other living processes - including the Divine!
  • And secondly, that God's Loving Self coupled with divine freedom gave birth to creation alive with possibility... including that of failure, death, and sin. Of which this living, organic constitution can, and will, admit sin and evil, as very real elements to it's inheritance. That not only life, but death, has come with the gift of God's Love on multiply levels and in multiple fashions.
  • And though some say God commanded freewill by divine fiat, a good process theology will say that creational freewill came by the very act of divine Self-giving birthing life by divine Love and not by divine fiat. Because if God had done so by divine command then it would make of God a divine monster and our lives as governed by fate and fortune rather than as by divine Love.
  • Therefore, with God's Love (and Loving act of creation) came uncontrollable (e.g., indeterminant) freewill. And with this birth also came the added problem of love's passion and freewill's waywardness. Sometimes, I think of God's Loving gift of God's Self as a Pandora's Box of divine gifts "hard to hold".

For now, let me end here. Our next post will require re-reading yesterday's articles and vids and the Wikipedia article here below.

As always,

Blessings,

R.E. Slater
March 22, 2023




* * * * * * * * *


Pandora

Pandora, (Greek: “All-Gifts”) in Greek mythology, the first woman. According to Hesiod’s Theogony, after Prometheus, a fire god and divine trickster, had stolen fire from heaven and bestowed it upon mortals, Zeus, the king of the gods, determined to counteract this blessing. He accordingly commissioned Hephaestus (a god of fire and patron of craftsmen) to fashion a woman out of earth, upon whom the gods bestowed their choicest gifts. In Hesiod’s Works and Days, Pandora had a jar containing all manner of misery and evil. Zeus sent her to Epimetheus, who forgot the warning of his brother Prometheus and made Pandora his wife. She afterward opened the jar, from which the evils flew out over the earth. Hope alone remained inside, the lid having been shut down before she could escape. In a later story the jar contained not evils but blessings, which would have been preserved for the human race had they not been lost through the opening of the jar out of curiosity. Pandora’s jar became a box in the 16th century, when the Renaissance humanist Erasmus either mistranslated the Greek or confused the vessel with the box in the story of Cupid and Psyche


* * * * * * * * *

Evolutionary origin of religion

The evolutionary origin of religion and religious behavior is a field of study related to evolutionary psychology, the origin of language and mythology, and cross-cultural comparison of the anthropology of religion. Some subjects of interest include Neolithic religion, evidence for spirituality or cultic behavior in the Upper Paleolithic, and similarities in great ape behavior.

Nonhuman religious behavior

Humanity's closest living relatives are common chimpanzees and bonobos.[1][2] These primates share a common ancestor with humans who lived between six and eight million years ago. It is for this reason that chimpanzees and bonobos are viewed as the best available surrogate for this common ancestor. Barbara King argues that while non-human primates are not religious, they do exhibit some traits that would have been necessary for the evolution of religion. These traits include high intelligence, a capacity for symbolic communication, a sense of social norms, and realization of "self" continuity.[3][4] There is inconclusive evidence that Homo neanderthalensis may have buried their dead, which would be evidence of mortuary ritual.[citation needed] The use of burial rituals is thought to be evidence of religious activity, but there is no other evidence that religion existed in human culture before humans reached behavioral modernity.[5] Other lines of evidence have revealed that Homo neanderthalensis made cave art, which would require a high level of symbolic thinking paralleling religious thought.[6]

Elephants perform rituals for their dead. They demonstrate long periods of silence and mourning at the point of death; later, elephants return to grave sites and caress the remains.[7][8] Some evidence suggests that many species grieve death and loss.[9]

Relevant prerequisites for human religion

Increased brain size

In this set of theories, the religious mind is one consequence of a brain that is large enough to formulate religious and philosophical ideas.[10] During human evolution, the hominid brain tripled in size, peaking 500,000 years ago. Much of the brain's expansion took place in the neocortex. The cerebral neocortex is presumed to be responsible for the neural computations underlying complex phenomena such as perception, thought, language, attention, episodic memory and voluntary movement.[11] According to Dunbar's theory, the relative neocortex size of any species correlates with the level of social complexity of the particular species.[12] The neocortex size correlates with a number of social variables that include social group size and complexity of mating behaviors.[13] In chimpanzees the neocortex occupies 50% of the brain, whereas in modern humans it occupies 80% of the brain.[14]

Robin Dunbar argues that the critical event in the evolution of the neocortex took place at the speciation of archaic Homo sapiens about 500,000 years ago. His study indicates that only after the speciation event is the neocortex large enough to process complex social phenomena such as language and religion. The study is based on a regression analysis of neocortex size plotted against a number of social behaviors of living and extinct hominids.[15]

Stephen Jay Gould suggests that religion may have grown out of evolutionary changes which favored larger brains as a means of cementing group coherence among savanna hunters, after that larger brain enabled reflection on the inevitability of personal mortality.[16]

Tool use

Lewis Wolpert argues that causal beliefs that emerged from tool use played a major role in the evolution of belief. The manufacture of complex tools requires creating a mental image of an object which does not exist naturally before actually making the artifact. Furthermore, one must understand how the tool would be used, that requires an understanding of causality.[17] Accordingly, the level of sophistication of stone tools is a useful indicator of causal beliefs.[18] Wolpert contends use of tools composed of more than one component, such as hand axes, represents an ability to understand cause and effect. However, recent studies of other primates indicate that causality may not be a uniquely human trait. For example, chimpanzees have been known to escape from pens closed with multiple latches, which was previously thought could only have been figured out by humans who understood causality. Chimpanzees are also known to mourn the dead, and notice things that have only aesthetic value, like sunsets, both of which may be considered to be components of religion or spirituality.[19][unreliable source?] The difference between the comprehension of causality by humans and chimpanzees is one of degree. The degree of comprehension in an animal depends upon the size of the prefrontal cortex: the greater the size of the prefrontal cortex the deeper the comprehension.[20][unreliable source?]

Development of language

Religion requires a system of symbolic communication, such as language, to be transmitted from one individual to another. Philip Lieberman states "human religious thought and moral sense clearly rest on a cognitive-linguistic base".[21] From this premise science writer Nicholas Wade states:

"Like most behaviors that are found in societies throughout the world, religion must have been present in the ancestral human population before the dispersal from Africa 50,000 years ago. Although religious rituals usually involve dance and music, they are also very verbal, since the sacred truths have to be stated. If so, religion, at least in its modern form, cannot pre-date the emergence of language. It has been argued earlier that language attained its modern state shortly before the exodus from Africa. If religion had to await the evolution of modern, articulate language, then it too would have emerged shortly before 50,000 years ago."[22]

Another view distinguishes individual religious belief from collective religious belief. While the former does not require prior development of language, the latter does. The individual human brain has to explain a phenomenon in order to comprehend and relate to it. This activity predates by far the emergence of language and may have caused it. The theory is, belief in the supernatural emerges from hypotheses arbitrarily assumed by individuals to explain natural phenomena that cannot be explained otherwise. The resulting need to share individual hypotheses with others leads eventually to collective religious belief. A socially accepted hypothesis becomes dogmatic backed by social sanction.

Language consists of digital contrasts whose cost is essentially zero. As pure social conventions, signals of this kind cannot evolve in a Darwinian social world—they are a theoretical impossibility.[23][24] Being intrinsically unreliable, language works only if one can build up a reputation for trustworthiness within a certain kind of society—namely, one where symbolic cultural facts (sometimes called 'institutional facts') can be established and maintained through collective social endorsement.[25] In any hunter-gatherer society, the basic mechanism for establishing trust in symbolic cultural facts is collective ritual.[26]

Transcending the continuity-versus-discontinuity divide, some scholars view the emergence of language as the consequence of some kind of social transformation[27] that, by generating unprecedented levels of public trust, liberated a genetic potential for linguistic creativity that had previously lain dormant.[28][29][30] "Ritual/speech coevolution theory" exemplifies this approach.[31][32] Scholars in this intellectual camp point to the fact that even chimpanzees and bonobos have latent symbolic capacities that they rarely—if ever—use in the wild.[33] Objecting to the sudden mutation idea, these authors argue that even if a chance mutation were to install a language organ in an evolving bipedal primate, it would be adaptively useless under all known primate social conditions. A very specific social structure—one capable of upholding unusually high levels of public accountability and trust—must have evolved before or concurrently with language to make reliance on "cheap signals" (words) an evolutionarily stable strategy. The animistic nature of early human language could serve as the handicap-like cost that helped to ensure the reliability of communication. The attribution of spiritual essence to everything surrounding early humans served as a built-in mechanism that provided instant verification and ensured the inviolability of one's speech.[34]

Animal vocal signals are, for the most part, intrinsically reliable. When a cat purrs, the signal constitutes direct evidence of the animal's contented state. The signal is trusted, not because the cat is inclined to be honest, but because it just cannot fake that sound. Primate vocal calls may be slightly more manipulable, but they remain reliable for the same reason—because they are hard to fake.[35] Primate social intelligence is "Machiavellian"—self-serving and unconstrained by moral scruples. Monkeys and apes often attempt to deceive each other, while at the same time remaining constantly on guard against falling victim to deception themselves.[36][37] Paradoxically, it is theorized that primates' resistance to deception is what blocks the evolution of their signalling systems along language-like lines. Language is ruled out because the best way to guard against being deceived is to ignore all signals except those that are instantly verifiable. Words automatically fail this test.[31]

Morality and group living

Frans de Waal and Barbara King both view human morality as having grown out of primate sociality. Although morality awareness may be a unique human trait, many social animals, such as primates, dolphins and whales, have been known to exhibit pre-moral sentiments. According to Michael Shermer, the following characteristics are shared by humans and other social animals, particularly the great apes:

attachment and bonding, cooperation and mutual aid, sympathy and empathy, direct and indirect reciprocity, altruism and reciprocal altruism, conflict resolution and peacemaking, deception and deception detection, community concern and caring about what others think about you, and awareness of and response to the social rules of the group.[38]

De Waal contends that all social animals have had to restrain or alter their behavior for group living to be worthwhile. Pre-moral sentiments evolved in primate societies as a method of restraining individual selfishness and building more cooperative groups. For any social species, the benefits of being part of an altruistic group should outweigh the benefits of individualism. For example, a lack of group cohesion could make individuals more vulnerable to attack from outsiders. Being part of a group may also improve the chances of finding food. This is evident among animals that hunt in packs to take down large or dangerous prey.

All social animals have hierarchical societies in which each member knows its own place. Social order is maintained by certain rules of expected behavior and dominant group members enforce order through punishment. However, higher order primates also have a sense of fairness. [39]

Chimpanzees live in fission-fusion groups that average 50 individuals. It is likely that early ancestors of humans lived in groups of similar size. Based on the size of extant hunter-gatherer societies, recent Paleolithic hominids lived in bands of a few hundred individuals. As community size increased over the course of human evolution, greater enforcement to achieve group cohesion would have been required. Morality may have evolved in these bands of 100 to 200 people as a means of social control, conflict resolution and group solidarity. According to Dr. de Waal, human morality has two extra levels of sophistication that are not found in primate societies.

Psychologist Matt J. Rossano argues that religion emerged after morality and built upon morality by expanding the social scrutiny of individual behavior to include supernatural agents. By including ever-watchful ancestors, spirits and gods in the social realm, humans discovered an effective strategy for restraining selfishness and building more cooperative groups.[40] The adaptive value of religion would have enhanced group survival.[41][42] Rossano is referring here to collective religious belief and the social sanction that institutionalized morality. According to Rossano's teaching, individual religious belief is thus initially epistemological, not ethical, in nature.

Evolutionary psychology of religion

Cognitive scientists underlined that religions may be explained as a result of the brain architecture that developed early in the genus Homo in the course of the evolutionary history of life. However, there is disagreement on the exact mechanisms that drove the evolution of the religious mind. The two main schools of thought hold:

  • either that religion evolved due to natural selection and has selective advantage
  • or that religion is an evolutionary byproduct of other mental adaptations.

Stephen Jay Gould, for example, saw religion as an exaptation or a spandrel, in other words: religion evolved as byproduct of psychological mechanisms that evolved for other reasons.[43][44][unreliable source?][45]

Such mechanisms may include the ability to infer the presence of organisms that might do harm (agent detection), the ability to come up with causal narratives for natural events (etiology), and the ability to recognize that other people have minds of their own with their own beliefs, desires and intentions (theory of mind). These three adaptations (among others) allow human beings to imagine purposeful agents behind many observations that could not readily be explained otherwise, e.g. thunder, lightning, movement of planets, complexity of life.[46] The emergence of collective religious belief identified such agents as deities that standardized the explanation.[47]

Some scholars have suggested that religion is genetically "hardwired" into the human condition. One controversial proposal, the God gene hypothesis, states that some variants of a specific gene, the VMAT2 gene, predispose to spirituality.[48]

Another view builds on the concept of the triune brain: the reptilian brain, the limbic system, and the neocortex, proposed by Paul D. MacLean. Collective religious belief draws upon the emotions of love, fear, and gregariousness and is deeply embedded in the limbic system through socio-biological conditioning and social sanction. Individual religious belief utilizes reason based in the neocortex and often varies from collective religion. The limbic system is much older in evolutionary terms than the neocortex and is, therefore, stronger than it – much in the same way as the reptilian is stronger than both the limbic system and the neocortex.

Yet another view is that the behavior of people who participate in a religion makes them feel better and this improves their biological fitness, so that there is a genetic selection in favor of people who are willing to believe in a religion. Specifically, rituals, beliefs, and the social contact typical of religious groups may serve to calm the mind (for example by reducing ambiguity and the uncertainty due to complexity) and allow it to function better when under stress.[49] This would allow religion to be used[by whom?] as a powerful survival mechanism, particularly in facilitating the evolution of hierarchies of warriors, which if true, may be why many modern religions tend to promote fertility and kinship.

Still another view, proposed by Fred H. Previc, sees human religion as a product of an increase in dopaminergic functions in the human brain and of a general intellectual expansion beginning around 80 thousand years ago (kya).[50][51][52] Dopamine promotes an emphasis on distant space and time, which can correlate with religious experience.[53] While the earliest extant shamanic cave-paintings date to around 40 kya, the use of ocher for rock art predates this and there is clear evidence for abstract thinking along the coast of South Africa 80 kya.

Paul Bloom suggests that "certain early emergent cognitive biases ... make it natural to believe in Gods and spirits".[54]

Prehistoric evidence of religion

The exact time when humans first became religious remains unknown, however research in evolutionary archaeology shows credible evidence of religious-cum-ritualistic behavior from around the Middle Paleolithic era (45–200 thousand years ago).[55]

Paleolithic burials

The earliest evidence of religious thought is based on the ritual treatment of the dead. Most animals display only a casual interest in the dead of their own species.[56][unreliable source?] Ritual burial thus represents a significant change in human behavior. Ritual burials represent an awareness of life and death and a possible belief in the afterlifePhilip Lieberman states "burials with grave goods clearly signify religious practices and concern for the dead that transcends daily life."[21]

The earliest evidence for treatment of the dead comes from Atapuerca in Spain. At this location the bones of 30 individuals believed to be Homo heidelbergensis have been found in a pit.[57] Neanderthals are also contenders for the first hominids to intentionally bury the dead. They may have placed corpses into shallow graves along with stone tools and animal bones. The presence of these grave goods may indicate an emotional connection with the deceased and possibly a belief in the afterlife. Neanderthal burial sites include Shanidar in Iraq and Krapina in Croatia and Kebara Cave in Israel.[58][59]

The earliest known burial of modern humans is from a cave in Israel located at Qafzeh. Human remains have been dated to 100,000 years ago. Human skeletons were found stained with red ocher. A variety of grave goods were found at the burial site. The mandible of a wild boar was found placed in the arms of one of the skeletons.[60] Philip Lieberman states:

Burial rituals incorporating grave goods may have been invented by the anatomically modern hominids who emigrated from Africa to the Middle East roughly 100,000 years ago

Matt Rossano suggests that the period between 80,000 and 60,000 years before present, following the retreat of humans from the Levant to Africa, was a crucial period in the evolution of religion.[61]

Use of symbolism

The use of symbolism in religion is a universal established phenomenon. Archeologist Steven Mithen contends that it is common for religious practices to involve the creation of images and symbols to represent supernatural beings and ideas. Because supernatural beings violate the principles of the natural world, there will always be difficulty in communicating and sharing supernatural concepts with others. This problem can be overcome by anchoring these supernatural beings in material form through representational art. When translated into material form, supernatural concepts become easier to communicate and understand.[62][unreliable source?] Due to the association of art and religion, evidence of symbolism in the fossil record is indicative of a mind capable of religious thoughts. Art and symbolism demonstrates a capacity for abstract thought and imagination necessary to construct religious ideas. Wentzel van Huyssteen states that the translation of the non-visible through symbolism enabled early human ancestors to hold beliefs in abstract terms.[63]

Some of the earliest evidence of symbolic behavior is associated with Middle Stone Age sites in Africa. From at least 100,000 years ago, there is evidence of the use of pigments such as red ocher. Pigments are of little practical use to hunter gatherers, thus evidence of their use is interpreted as symbolic or for ritual purposes. Among extant hunter gatherer populations around the world, red ocher is still used extensively for ritual purposes. It has been argued that it is universal among human cultures for the color red to represent blood, sex, life and death.[64]

The use of red ocher as a proxy for symbolism is often criticized as being too indirect. Some scientists, such as Richard Klein and Steven Mithen, only recognize unambiguous forms of art as representative of abstract ideas. Upper paleolithic cave art provides some of the most unambiguous evidence of religious thought from the paleolithic. Cave paintings at Chauvet depict creatures that are half human and half animal.

Origins of organized religion

Social evolution of humans[38][65]
Period years agoSociety typeNumber of individuals
100,000–10,000Bands10s–100s
10,000–5,000Tribes100s–1,000s
5,000–3,000Chiefdoms1,000s–10,000s
3,000–1,000States10,000s–100,000s
2,000*–presentEmpires100,000–1,000,000s

Organized religion traces its roots to the neolithic revolution that began 11,000 years ago in the Near East but may have occurred independently in several other locations around the world. The invention of agriculture transformed many human societies from a hunter-gatherer lifestyle to a sedentary lifestyle. The consequences of the neolithic revolution included a population explosion and an acceleration in the pace of technological development. The transition from foraging bands to states and empires precipitated more specialized and developed forms of religion that reflected the new social and political environment. While bands and small tribes possess supernatural beliefs, these beliefs do not serve to justify a central authority, justify transfer of wealth or maintain peace between unrelated individuals. Organized religion emerged as a means of providing social and economic stability through the following ways:

  • Justifying the central authority, which in turn possessed the right to collect taxes in return for providing social and security services.
  • Bands and tribes consist of small number of related individuals. However, states and nations are composed of many thousands of unrelated individuals. Jared Diamond argues that organized religion served to provide a bond between unrelated individuals who would otherwise be more prone to enmity. In his book Guns, Germs, and Steel he argues that the leading cause of death among hunter-gatherer societies is murder.[65]
  • Religions that revolved around moralizing gods may have facilitated the rise of large, cooperative groups of unrelated individuals.[66]

The states born out of the Neolithic revolution, such as those of Ancient Egypt and Mesopotamia, were theocracies with chiefs, kings and emperors playing dual roles of political and spiritual leaders.[38] Anthropologists have found that virtually all state societies and chiefdoms from around the world have been found to justify political power through divine authority. This suggests that political authority co-opts collective religious belief to bolster itself.[38]

Invention of writing

Following the neolithic revolution, the pace of technological development (cultural evolution) intensified due to the invention of writing 5,000 years ago. Symbols that became words later on made effective communication of ideas possible. Printing invented only over a thousand years ago increased the speed of communication exponentially and became the main spring of cultural evolution. Writing is thought to have been first invented in either Sumeria or Ancient Egypt and was initially used for accounting. Soon after, writing was used to record myth. The first religious texts mark the beginning of religious history. The Pyramid Texts from ancient Egypt are one of the oldest known religious texts in the world, dating to between 2400 and 2300 BCE.[67][68][69][unreliable source?] Writing played a major role in sustaining and spreading organized religion. In pre-literate societies, religious ideas were based on an oral tradition, the contents of which were articulated by shamans and remained limited to the collective memories of the society's inhabitants. With the advent of writing, information that was not easy to remember could easily be stored in sacred texts that were maintained by a select group (clergy). Humans could store and process large amounts of information with writing that otherwise would have been forgotten. Writing therefore enabled religions to develop coherent and comprehensive doctrinal systems that remained independent of time and place.[70] Writing also brought a measure of objectivity to human knowledge. Formulation of thoughts in words and the requirement for validation made mutual exchange of ideas and the sifting of generally acceptable from not acceptable ideas possible. The generally acceptable ideas became objective knowledge reflecting the continuously evolving framework of human awareness of reality that Karl Popper calls 'verisimilitude' – a stage on the human journey to truth.[71]

See also

References

  1. ^ Gibbons, Ann (June 13, 2012). "Bonobos Join Chimps as Closest Human Relatives"Archived from the original on June 20, 2018. Retrieved July 19, 2018.
  2. ^ Ajit Varki; Daniel H. Geschwind; Evan E. Eichler (October 1, 2008). "Explaining human uniqueness: genome interactions with environment, behaviour and culture"Nature Reviews Genetics9 (10): 3, 51–54. doi:10.1038/nrg2428PMC 2756412PMID 18802414.
  3. ^ King, Barbara (2007). Evolving God: A Provocative View on the Origins of Religion. Doubleday Publishing. ISBN 0-385-52155-3.
  4. ^ Barbara J. King (2007), Excerpted from Evolving God, archived from the original on January 18, 2008
  5. ^ Palmer, Douglas, Simon Lamb, Guerrero Angeles. Gavira, and Peter Frances. Prehistoric Life: the Definitive Visual History of Life on Earth. New York, N.Y.: DK Pub., 2009.
  6. ^ Hoffmann, D. L.; Standish, C. D.; García-Diez, M.; Pettitt, P. B.; Milton, J. A.; Zilhão, J.; Alcolea-González, J. J.; Cantalejo-Duarte, P.; Collado, H.; de Balbín, R.; Lorblanchet, M.; Ramos-Muñoz, J.; Weniger, G.-Ch.; Pike, A. W. G. (February 23, 2018). "U-Th dating of carbonate crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art" (PDF)Science. American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS). 359 (6378): 912–915. Bibcode:2018Sci...359..912Hdoi:10.1126/science.aap7778ISSN 0036-8075PMID 29472483S2CID 206664238.
  7. ^ Bhattacharya, Shaoni (October 26, 2005). "Elephants may pay homage to dead relatives"Biology Letters2 (1): 26–28. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0400PMC 1617198PMID 17148317. Retrieved March 11, 2016.
  8. ^ McComb, K.; Baker, L.; Moss, C. (March 22, 2006). "African elephants show high levels of interest in the skulls and ivory of their own species | Biology Letters"Biology Letters2 (1): 26–28. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0400PMC 1617198PMID 17148317.
  9. ^ Bekoff, Marc (October 29, 2009). "Grief in animals: It's arrogant to think we're the only animals who mourn"Psychology Today.
  10. ^ Ehrlich, Paul (2000). Human Natures: Genes, Cultures, and the Human Prospect. Washington, D.C.: Island Press. p. 214ISBN 978-1-55963-779-4Religious ideas can theoretically be traced to the evolution of brains large enough to make possible the kind of abstract thought necessary to formulate religious and philosophical ideas
  11. ^ Molnár, Zoltán; Pollen, Alex (January 1, 2014). "How unique is the human neocortex?"Development141 (1): 11–16. doi:10.1242/dev.101279PMID 24346696.
  12. ^ Dávid-Barrett, T.; Dunbar, R. I. M. (August 22, 2013). "Processing power limits social group size: computational evidence for the cognitive costs of sociality"Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences280 (1765): 20131151. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.1151ISSN 0962-8452PMC 3712454PMID 23804623.
  13. ^ Dunbar, Robin I. M.; Shultz, Susanne (June 1, 2010). "Bondedness and sociality". Behaviour147 (7): 775–803. doi:10.1163/000579510X501151ISSN 1568-539X.
  14. ^ "9 Brainy Facts About the Neocortex"mentalfloss.com. November 17, 2016. Retrieved March 20, 2018.
  15. ^ Dunbar, R.I.M. (2003). "The Social Brain: Mind, Language, and Society in Evolutionary Perspective" (PDF)Annual Review of Anthropology. Annual Reviews. 32 (1): 163–181. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158ISSN 0084-6570. Archived from the original (PDF) on September 11, 2008.
  16. ^ Stephen Jay Gould; Paul McGarr; Steven Peter Russell Rose (2007). "Challenges to Neo-Darwinism and Their Meaning for a Revised View of Human Consciousness"The richness of life: the essential Stephen Jay Gould. W. W. Norton & Company. pp. 232–233ISBN 978-0-393-06498-8.
  17. ^ Lewis Wolpert (2006). Six impossible things before breakfast, The evolutionary origins of belief. New York: Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-06449-0with regard to hafted tools, One would have to understand that the two pieces serve different purposes, and imagine how the tool could be used,
  18. ^ Wolpert, Lewis (2006). Six impossible things before breakfast, The evolutionary origins of belief. New York: Norton. p. 82ISBN 978-0-393-06449-0Belief in cause and effect has had the most enormous effect on human evolution, both physical and cultural. Tool use, with language, has transformed human evolution and let to what we now think of as belief
  19. ^ Connor, Steve (August 5, 2011). "Can an ape learn to be human?"The Independent. London.
  20. ^ Mohandas, E (2008). "Neurobiology of spirituality"Mens Sana Monogr6 (1): 63–80. doi:10.4103/0973-1229.33001PMC 3190564PMID 22013351.
  21. Jump up to:a b Lieberman (1991). Uniquely Human. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-92183-2.
  22. ^ Wade, Nicholas (2006). Before the Dawn, Discovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors. Penguin Books, London. p. 8, p. 165. ISBN 1-59420-079-3
  23. ^ Zahavi, A. (May 1993). "The fallacy of conventional signalling". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences340 (1292): 227–230. Bibcode:1993RSPTB.340..227Zdoi:10.1098/rstb.1993.0061PMID 8101657.
  24. ^ Zahavi, A. and A. Zahavi 1997. The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece in Darwin's Puzzle. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780190284589
  25. ^ Searle, J. R. 1996. The Construction of Social Reality. London: Penguin.
  26. ^ Durkheim, E. 1947 [1915]. "Origins of these beliefs". Chapter VII. In É. Durkheim, The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life: A study in religious sociology. Trans. J. W. Swain. Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, pp. 205–239.
  27. ^ Knight, Chris; Power, Camilla (2012). Maggie Tallerman; Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.). Social conditions for the evolutionary emergence of language (PDF)The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 346–49. ISBN 978-0-19-954111-9OCLC 724665645.
  28. ^ Rappaport, Roy (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge, U.K. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521296908OCLC 848728046.
  29. ^ Knight, C. (2008). "'Honest fakes' and language origins" (PDF)Journal of Consciousness Studies15 (10–11): 236–48.
  30. ^ Knight, Chris (2010). Ulrich J Frey; Charlotte Störmer; Kai P Willführ (eds.). The origins of symbolic culture (PDF)Homo novus: a human without illusion. Berlin; New York: Springer. pp. 193–211. ISBN 978-3-642-12141-8OCLC 639461749.
  31. Jump up to:a b Knight, Chris (1998). James R Hurford; Michael Studdert-Kennedy; Chris Knight (eds.). Ritual/speech coevolution: a solution to the problem of deception (PDF)Approaches to the evolution of language: social and cognitive base. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 68–91. ISBN 978-0-521-63964-4OCLC 37742390.
  32. ^ Knight, Chris (2006). Angelo Cangelosi; Andrew D M Smith; Kenny Smith (eds.). Language co-evolved with the rule of law (PDF)The evolution of language: proceedings of the 6th international conference (EVOLANG6), Rome, Italy, 12–15 April 200. New Jersey: World Scientific Publishing. pp. 168–175. ISBN 978-981-256-656-0OCLC 70797781.
  33. ^ Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue; McDonald, Kelly (1988). Richard W Byrne; Andrew Whiten (eds.). Deception and social manipulation in symbol-using apesMachiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and human. Oxford: Clarendon Press. pp. 224–237. ISBN 978-0-19-852175-4OCLC 17260831.
  34. ^ Hasanov, Eldar T. (2021). "Animism and Language Evolution"SSRN Electronic Journal. Elsevier BV. doi:10.2139/ssrn.3944281ISSN 1556-5068S2CID 244431830.
  35. ^ Goodall, Jane (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-11649-8OCLC 12550961.
  36. ^ Byrne, Richard W.; Whiten, Andrew. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press. ISBN 978-0-19-852175-4OCLC 17260831.
  37. ^ de Waal, Frans B. M. (2005). "Intentional Deception in Primates". Evolutionary Anthropology1 (3): 86–92. doi:10.1002/evan.1360010306S2CID 221736130.
  38. Jump up to:a b c d Shermer, Michael (2004). "Why are we moral:The evolutionary origins of morality"The Science of Good and Evil. New York: Times BooksISBN 978-0-8050-7520-5.
  39. ^ Landau, Elizabeth (January 19, 2013). "Morality: It's not just for humans"cnn.com. CNN. Retrieved March 20, 2018.
  40. ^ Rossano, Matt (2007). "Supernaturalizing Social Life: Religion and the Evolution of Human Cooperation" (PDF)Human Nature18 (3): 272–94. doi:10.1007/s12110-007-9002-4PMID 26181064S2CID 1585551.
  41. ^ Wade, Nicholas (March 20, 2007). "Scientist Finds the Beginnings of Morality in Primate Behavior"The New York Times.
  42. ^ Matthew Rutherford. The Evolution of Morality. University of Glasgow. 2007. Retrieved June 6, 2008
  43. ^ "Darwin's God"The New York Times Magazine. March 4, 2007.
  44. ^ Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality Archived September 10, 2008, at the Wayback Machine
  45. ^ Steven Pinker (October 29, 2004). "The evolutionary psychology of religion"harvard.edu. Archived from the original on May 9, 2008. Retrieved March 20, 2018.
  46. ^ Atran, S; Norenzayan, A (2004). "Religion's evolutionary landscape: counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion". Behavioral and Brain Sciences27 (6): 713–30, discussion 730–70. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.687.8586doi:10.1017/s0140525x04000172PMID 16035401S2CID 1177255.
  47. ^ Dávid-Barrett, Tamás; Carney, James (August 14, 2015). "The deification of historical figures and the emergence of priesthoods as a solution to a network coordination problem". Religion, Brain & Behavior6 (4): 307–317. doi:10.1080/2153599X.2015.1063001ISSN 2153-599XS2CID 146979343.
  48. ^ Kluger, Jeffrey; Jeff Chu; Broward Liston; Maggie Sieger; Daniel Williams (October 25, 2004). "Is God in our genes?"Time. Archived from the original on September 30, 2007. Retrieved April 8, 2007.
  49. ^ Lionel Tiger and Michael McGuire (2010). God's Brain. Prometheus Books. pp. 202–204. ISBN 978-1-61614-164-6.
  50. ^ Previc, F.H. (2009). The dopaminergic mind in human evolution and history. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  51. ^ Previc, F.H. (2011). Dopamine, altered consciousness, and distant space with special reference to shamanic ecstasy. In E. Cardona & M. Winkelman (eds.), Altering consciousness: Multidisciplinary perspectives (Vol. 1), pp. 43–60. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO, LLC.
  52. ^ Previc, Fred H (2006). "The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity". Consciousness and Cognition15 (3): 500–39. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.009PMID 16439158S2CID 16239814.
  53. ^ Previc, Fred H. (2006). "The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity". Consciousness and Cognition15 (3): 500–539. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.009PMID 16439158S2CID 16239814Hyperreligiosity is a major feature of mania, obsessive-compulsive disorder, schizophrenia, temporal-lobe epilepsy and related disorders, in which the ventromedial dopaminergic systems are highly activated and exaggerated attentional or goal-directed behavior toward extrapersonal space occurs. The evolution of religion is linked to an expansion of dopaminergic systems in humans, brought about by changes in diet and other physiological influences.
  54. ^ Shah, Timothy Samuel; Friedman, Jack, eds. (January 11, 2018). Homo Religiosus?: Exploring the Roots of Religion and Religious Freedom in Human Experience. Cambridge Studies in Religion, Philosophy, and Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (published 2018). p. 1. ISBN 9781108422352. Retrieved September 4, 2021cognitive scientist Paul Bloom ... holds that 'there are certain early emergent cognitive biases that make it natural to believe in Gods and spirits, in an afterlife, and in the divine creation of the universe'
  55. ^ Culotta, Elizabeth (November 6, 2009). "On the Origin of Religion". Science326 (5954): 784–787. Bibcode:2009Sci...326..784Cdoi:10.1126/science.326_784PMID 19892955.
  56. ^ Elephants may pay homage to the dead
  57. ^ Greenspan, Stanley (2006). How Symbols, Language, and Intelligence Evolved from Early Primates to Modern Human. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press. ISBN 978-0-306-81449-5.
  58. ^ "The Neanderthal dead:exploring mortuary variability in Middle Palaeolithic Eurasia" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on September 10, 2008.[clarification needed]
  59. ^ "BBC article on the Neanderthals"Neanderthals buried their dead, and one burial at Shanidar in Iraq was accompanied by grave goods in the form of plants. All of the plants are used in recent times for medicinal purposes, and it seems likely that the Neanderthals also used them in this way and buried them with their dead for the same reason. Grave goods are an archaeological marker of belief in an afterlife, so Neanderthals may well have had some form of religious belief.
  60. ^ Uniquely Human page 163
  61. ^ Rossano, Matt (2009). The African Interregnum: The "Where," "When," and "Why" of the Evolution of Religion (PDF). The Frontiers Collection. pp. 127–141. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-00128-4_9ISBN 978-3-642-00127-7.
  62. ^ Symbolism and the Supernatural
  63. ^ "Human Uniqueness and Symbolization". Archived from the original on May 27, 2009. Retrieved August 24, 2008This 'coding of the non-visible' through abstract, symbolic thought, enabled also our early human ancestors to argue and hold beliefs in abstract terms. In fact, the concept of God itself follows from the ability to abstract and conceive of 'person'
  64. ^ Rossano, Matt J. (2006). "The Religious Mind and the Evolution of Religion" (PDF)Review of General Psychology. SAGE Publications. 10 (4): 346–364. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.346ISSN 1089-2680S2CID 8490298.
  65. Jump up to:a b Diamond, Jared (1997). "Chapter 14, From Egalitarianism to Kleptocracy"Guns Germs and Steel. New York, NY: Norton. p. 277ISBN 978-0-393-03891-0.
  66. ^ Norenzayan, A.; Shariff, A. F. (2008). "The origin and evolution of religious prosociality". Science322 (5898): 58–62. Bibcode:2008Sci...322...58NCiteSeerX 10.1.1.659.6887doi:10.1126/science.1158757PMID 18832637S2CID 28514.
  67. ^ Budge, Wallis (1997). An Introduction to Ancient Egyptian Literature. Mineola, N.Y.: Dover Publications. p. 9. ISBN 978-0-486-29502-2.
  68. ^ Allen, James (2007). The Ancient Egyptian Pyramid Texts. Atlanta, Ga.: Scholars Press. ISBN 978-1-58983-182-7.
  69. ^ The beginning of religion at the beginning of the Neolithic Archived September 10, 2008, at the Wayback Machine
  70. ^ Pyysiäinen, Ilkka (2004). "Holy Book:The Invention of writing and religious cognition"Magic, Miracles, and Religion: A Scientist's Perspective. Walnut Creek, CA: AltMira Press. ISBN 978-0-7591-0663-5.
  71. ^ Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach, 1972, Rev. ed., 1979, ISBN 0-19-875024-2

Bibliography

  • Pascal Boyer, Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought, New York: Basic Books 2001.

External links